简介
首页

The Evolutionist at Large

I. MICROSCOPIC BRAINS.
关灯
护眼
字体:
上一章    回目录 下一章

sitting on this little rounded boss of gneiss beside the path which cuts obliquely through the meadow, i am engaged in watching a brigade of ants out on foraging duty, and intent on securing for the nest three whole segments of a deceased earthworm. they look for all the world like those busy companies one sees in the egyptian wall-paintings, dragging home a huge granite colossus by sheer force of bone and sinew. every muscle in their tiny bodies is strained to the utmost as they prise themselves laboriously against the great boulders which strew the path, and which are known to our brobdingnagian intelligence as grains of sand. besides the workers themselves, a whole battalion of stragglers runs to and fro upon the broad line which leads to the head-quarters of the community. the province of these stragglers, who seem so busy doing nothing, probably consists in keeping communications open, and encouraging the sturdy pullers by occasional relays of fresh workmen. i often wish that i could for a while get inside those tiny brains, and see, or rather smell, the world as ants do. for there can be little doubt that to these brave little carnivores here the universe is chiefly known as a collective bundle of odours, simultaneous or consecutive. as our world is mainly a world of visible objects, theirs, i believe, is mainly a world of olfactible things.

in the head of every one of these little creatures is something that we may fairly call a brain. of course most insects have no real brains; the nerve-substance in their heads is a mere collection of ill-arranged ganglia, directly connected with their organs of sense. whatever man may be, an earwig at least is a conscious, or rather a semi-conscious, automaton. he has just a few knots of nerve-cells in his little pate, each of which leads straight from his dim eye or his vague ear or his indefinite organs of taste; and his muscles obey the promptings of external sensations without possibility of hesitation or consideration, as mechanically as the valve of a steam-engine obeys the governor-balls. you may say of him truly, 'nihil est in intellectu quod non fuerit in sensu;' and you need not even add the leibnitzian saving clause, 'nisi ipse intellectus;' for the poor soul's intellect is wholly deficient, and the senses alone make up all that there is of him, subjectively considered. but it is not so with the highest insects. they have something which truly answers to the real brain of men, apes, and dogs, to the cerebral hemispheres and the cerebellum which are superadded in us mammals upon the simple sense-centres of lower creatures. besides the eye, with its optic nerve and optic perceptive organs—besides the ear, with its similar mechanism—we mammalian lords of creation have a higher and more genuine brain, which collects and compares the information given to the senses, and sends down the appropriate messages to the muscles accordingly. now, bees and flies and ants have got much the same sort of arrangement, on a smaller scale, within their tiny heads. on top of the little knots which do duty as nerve-centres for their eyes and mouths, stand two stalked bits of nervous matter, whose duty is analogous to that of our own brains. and that is why these three sorts of insects think and reason so much more intellectually than beetles or butterflies, and why the larger part of them have organised their domestic arrangements on such an excellent co-operative plan.

we know well enough what forms the main material of thought with bees and flies, and that is visible objects. for you must think about something if you think at all; and you can hardly imagine a contemplative blow-fly setting itself down to reflect, like a hindu devotee, on the syllable om, or on the oneness of existence. abstract ideas are not likely to play a large part in apian consciousness. a bee has a very perfect eye, and with this eye it can see not only form, but also colour, as sir john lubbock's experiments have shown us. the information which it gets through its eye, coupled with other ideas derived from touch, smell, and taste, no doubt makes up the main thinkable and knowable universe as it reveals itself to the apian intelligence. to ourselves and to bees alike the world is, on the whole, a coloured picture, with the notions of distance and solidity thrown in by touch and muscular effort; but sight undoubtedly plays the first part in forming our total conception of things generally.

what, however, forms the thinkable universe of these little ants running to and fro so eagerly at my feet? that is a question which used long to puzzle me in my afternoon walks. the ant has a brain and an intelligence, but that brain and that intelligence must have been developed out of something. ex nihilo nihil fit. you cannot think and know if you have nothing to think about. the intelligence of the bee and the fly was evolved in the course of their flying about and looking at things: the more they flew, and the more they saw, the more they knew; and the more brain they got to think with. but the ant does not generally fly, and, as with most comparatively unlocomotive animals, its sight is bad. true, the winged males and females have retained in part the usual sharp eyes of their class—for they are first cousins to the bees—and they also possess three little eyelets or ocelli, which are wanting to the wingless neuters. without these they would never have found one another in their courtship, and they would have run their heads against the nearest tree, or rushed down the gaping throat of the first expectant swallow, and so effectually extinguished their race. flying animals cannot do without eyes, and they always possess the most highly developed vision of any living creatures. but the wingless neuters are almost blind—in some species quite so; and sir john lubbock has shown that their appreciation of colour is mostly confined to an aversion to red light, and a comparative endurance of blue. moreover, they are apparently deaf, and most of their other senses seem little developed. what can be the raw material on which that pin's head of a brain sets itself working? for, small as it is, it is a wonderful organ of intellect; and though sir john lubbock has shown us all too decisively that the originality and inventive genius of ants have been sadly overrated by solomon and others, yet darwin is probably right none the less in saying that no more marvellous atom of matter exists in the universe than this same wee lump of microscopic nerve substance.

my dog grip, running about on the path there, with his nose to the ground, and sniffing at every stick and stone he meets on his way, gives us the clue to solve the problem. grip, as professor croom robertson suggests, seems capable of extracting a separate and distinguishable smell from everything. i have only to shy a stone on the beach among a thousand other stones, and my dog, like a well-bred retriever as he is, selects and brings back to me that individual stone from all the stones around, by exercise of his nose alone. it is plain that grip's world is not merely a world of sights, but a world of smells as well. he not only smells smells, but he remembers smells, he thinks smells, he even dreams smells, as you may see by his sniffing and growling in his sleep. now, if i were to cut open grip's head (which heaven forfend), i should find in it a correspondingly big smell-nerve and smell-centre—an olfactory lobe, as the anatomists say. all the accumulated nasal experiences of his ancestors have made that lobe enormously developed. but in a man's head you would find a very large and fine optic centre, and only a mere shrivelled relic to represent the olfactory lobes. you and i and our ancestors have had but little occasion for sniffing and scenting; our sight and our touch have done duty as chief intelligencers from the outer world; and the nerves of smell, with their connected centres, have withered away to the degenerate condition in which they now are. consequently, smell plays but a small part in our thought and our memories. the world that we know is chiefly a world of sights and touches. but in the brain of dog, or deer, or antelope, smell is a prevailing faculty; it colours all their ideas, and it has innumerable nervous connections with every part of their brain. the big olfactory lobes are in direct communication with a thousand other nerves; odours rouse trains of thought or powerful emotions in their minds just as visible objects do in our own.

now, in the dog or the horse sight and smell are equally developed; so that they probably think of most things about equally in terms of each. in ourselves, sight is highly developed, and smell is a mere relic; so that we think of most things in terms of sight alone, and only rarely, as with a rose or a lily, in terms of both. but in ants, on the contrary, smell is highly developed and sight a mere relic; so that they probably think of most things as smellable only, and very little as visible in form or colour. dr. bastian has shown that bees and butterflies are largely guided by scent; and though he is certainly wrong in supposing that sight has little to do with leading them to flowers (for if you cut off the bright-coloured corolla they will never discover the mutilated blossoms, even when they visit others on the same plant), yet the mere fact that so many flowers are scented is by itself enough to show that perfume has a great deal to do with the matter. in wingless ants, while the eyes have undergone degeneration, this high sense of smell has been continued and further developed, till it has become their principal sense-endowment, and the chief raw material of their intelligence. their active little brains are almost wholly engaged in correlating and co-ordinating smells with actions. their olfactory nerves give them nearly all the information they can gain about the external world, and their brains take in this information and work out the proper movements which it indicates. by smell they find their way about and carry on the business of their lives. just as you and i know the road from regent's circus to pall mall by visible signs of the street-corners and the duke of york's column, so these little ants know the way from the nest to the corpse of the dismembered worm by observing and remembering the smells which they met with on their way. see: i obliterate the track for an inch or two with my stick, and the little creatures go beside themselves with astonishment and dismay. they rush about wildly, inquiring of one another with their antenn? whether this is really doomsday, and whether the whole course of nature has been suddenly revolutionised. then, after a short consultation, they determine upon action; and every ant starts off in a different direction to hunt the lost track, head to the ground, exactly as a pointer hunts the missing trail of a bird or hare. each ventures an inch or so off, and then runs back to find the rest, for fear he should get isolated altogether. at last, after many failures, one lucky fellow hits upon the well-remembered train of scents, and rushes back leaving smell-tracks no doubt upon the soil behind him. the message goes quickly round from post to post, each sentry making passes with his antenn? to the next picket, and so sending on the news to the main body in the rear. within five minutes communications are re-established, and the precious bit of worm-meat continues triumphantly on its way along the recovered path. an ingenious writer would even have us believe that ants possess a scent-language of their own, and emit various odours from their antenn? which the other ants perceive with theirs, and recognise as distinct in meaning. be this as it may, you cannot doubt, if you watch them long, that scents and scents alone form the chief means by which they recollect and know one another, or the external objects with which they come in contact. the whole universe is clearly to them a complicated picture made up entirely of infinite interfusing smells.

上一章    回目录 下一章
阅读记录 书签 书架 返回顶部